Discriminatory Experiences Among Black Youth: How Encounters and Expectations Explain Emotional Well-Being (2024)

  • Journal List
  • Springer
  • PMC11126461

As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsem*nt of, or agreement with, the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more: PMC Disclaimer | PMC Copyright Notice

Discriminatory Experiences Among Black Youth: How Encounters and Expectations Explain Emotional Well-Being (1)

Prev Sci. 2024; 25(1): 31–43.

Published online 2023 Jun 17. doi:10.1007/s11121-023-01540-2

PMCID: PMC11126461

PMID: 37329411

Nicole M. Summers-Gabr,Discriminatory Experiences Among Black Youth: How Encounters and Expectations Explain Emotional Well-Being (2)1 Mikiko Sato,2 Sarah M. Chilenski,3 Francisco Villarruel,2 Paula Smith,4 Charles Henderson,5 Jeremiah Newell,6 Hilder Wilson,6 and Astrid Craig7

Author information Article notes Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

This article has been corrected. See Prev Sci. 2024 May 24; : .

Associated Data

Data Availability Statement

Abstract

Research suggests that encounters with racism are related to depression in Black youth. However, less is known about how experienced racial discrimination can influence other aspects of well-being among Black youth including their socio-emotional development and behavior. In addition, emerging literature highlights the critical ways anticipated racial discrimination may impact the emotional well-being of Black youth. To address these gaps, the current study assessed whether experienced discrimination was associated with higher levels of internalizing problems (anxiety/depression, suicidal thoughts) and lower levels of socio-emotional development (emotion regulation, prosocial behavior). We then tested whether expected discrimination contributed to similar patterns. Lastly, this study examined how age and gender moderated this relationship. Across eight schools in three communities, 1435 Black youth (56.57% female; 56.40% 10th grade) in 10th and 12th grades responded to the Youth Experience Survey. Using a series of hierarchical linear and hierarchical binary logistic regressions, results found that those who experienced racial discrimination and expected discrimination demonstrated higher internalizing problems and lower socio-emotional development; however, expected discrimination often accounted for more variance than experienced. These findings suggest the multifaceted influence both experienced and expected racial discrimination have on the well-being of Black youth and can provide important insights to community prevention systems.

Keywords: Discrimination, Black youth, Adolescence, Social-emotion development, Mental health

Introduction

The impact of systemic racism has been at the forefront of national discourse and public health, particularly for Black youth. There is a critical need to understand the experiences of Black youth and how systemic barriers affect their emotional well-being. Racism can have dire consequences on the mental health of youth (Kirkinis et al., 2018; Walker et al., 2017). As racial inequality continues, scholars are responsible for documenting the mental health disparities that affect Black youth. This study provides details about mental health and specific important mental health-related processes thought to be impacted by discrimination. Therefore, the first aim of this investigation was to examine whether experienced and anticipated racial discrimination were associated with high levels of internalizing problems, specifically depression/anxiety and suicidal ideation. The second aim was to explore the effect experienced and expected racial discrimination had on social-emotional development, specifically emotion regulation and prosocial behavior. Lastly, this study explored how age and gender moderated the relationship between experienced and anticipated discrimination and the aforementioned outcomes.

Perceived Racial Discrimination and Mental Health

Perceived racial discrimination, which is biased and unjust treatment based on one’s racial identity (Assari et al., 2019), can result in poor mental and physical health outcomes for Black youth (Kirkinis et al., 2018). More specifically, experienced discrimination has been associated with depressive symptoms (Seaton & Yip, 2009), decreased self-esteem (Seaton et al., 2010), and increased suicidal thoughts (Walker et al., 2014, 2017). Studies have shown the widespread prevalence of racial discrimination among Black youth; reports typically increased during middle and late adolescence (Umaña-Taylor, 2016). For instance, research suggests that during early adolescence anywhere from 10% (Nagata et al., 2021) to 38% (Roberts et al., 2012) of Black youth experienced at least some racial discrimination. However, during mid to late adolescence, the proportions were as low as 22% (McNeil Smith & Fincham, 2015), but leaned more toward higher proportions such as 62.5% (Sellers et al., 2006) or even 89% of youth (Roberts et al., 2012). The complexity in these issues is furthered by youths’ ability to perceive discrimination (Cunningham et al., 2019) and the increase in stereotyping of African Americans by White and Black youth (Copping et al., 2013) during adolescence. Therefore, both mental health problems as well as the ability to perceive racial aggressions increase during the same developmental period and likely reinforce each other, which ultimately further exacerbates mental health disparities in adulthood due to its cumulative consequences.

Despite the well-established relationship between discrimination and mental health, there are relatively few studies on the influence discrimination has on social-emotional development, such as emotion regulation and prosocial behavior (Riley et al., 2021). Because discrimination is an emotion-eliciting experience, it compels people who have experienced it to be adaptive (Riley et al., 2021). One challenge in parsing out the impact of racial discrimination on social-emotional development is that it can be considered both an outcome (e.g., Padilla‐Walker et al., 2012) and a protective factor (Raposa et al., 2016; Taylor et al., 2017; Troy & Mauss, 2011). For instance, in the context of discrimination and emotion regulation, that could mean that the better someone’s emotion control is, the less likely there is to have suicidal ideation in response to experiencing discrimination. However, that could also mean that the more discrimination someone experiences, the harder it is for an individual to control emotions like anger, sadness, or fear.

Understanding the direction of the relationship between discrimination and prosocial behaviors is also challenging to deconstruct. Prosocial behaviors are what foster positive relationships and without them could lead to marginalization and social exclusion (Davis & Carlo, 2019). In one study, Lozada et al. (2017) found that school-based discrimination actually increased prosocial behaviors in Black male adolescents and theorized it was because there is an expectation for youth to organize and act in their community. In another study, but with Latino youth, Davis et al. (2021) found that the direction of the relationship between discrimination and prosocial behavior depended on whether prosocial behaviors were conducted publicly or altruistically (Davis et al., 2021). As research continues to progress in this topic, it is important to continue considering the relationship between social-emotional development, such as prosocial development, and poor mental health outcomes because it is crucial to understand how discrimination impacts the whole child (Davis et al., 2021).

Anticipated Racial Discrimination and Well-Being

Extensive literature underpins the impact of experienced discrimination on mental health; however, less is known about how anticipated discrimination influences well-being. Anticipated or expectations of racial discrimination is the notion that one will be discriminated against because of one’s racial background (Herda, 2016). Anticipated racial discrimination derives from cultural racism. Williams et al. (2019) described cultural racism as “the instillation of the ideology of inferiority in the values, language, imagery, symbols, and unstated assumptions of the larger society” (p. 110). Furthermore, cultural racism constitutes a society where systemic racism reinforces macro- and micro-levels of discrimination. In other words, cultural racism may generate beliefs among people of color that magnify chronic fear, anticipation, and stress of societal prejudice and discrimination because of one’s racial identity. Scholars argue that in addition to experienced discrimination, more empirical studies on anticipated discrimination are needed to better understand the link between discrimination and well-being (Herda, 2016).

Recent studies indicated that anticipated racial discrimination has important implications for youth mental well-being. For instance, Zimmerman and Miller-Smith (2021) explored the relevance of anticipated racial discrimination for depression and suicidal behavior among a diverse youth sample from the Project on Human Development in Chicago Neighborhoods. They found that anticipated racial discrimination was positively associated with major depressive disorder and suicidal behavior. Additionally, Black and Hispanic youth were disproportionately exposed to racial discrimination, with Black youth reporting the highest level of experienced and anticipated discrimination followed by Hispanic youth. These findings suggest that anticipated racial discrimination is highly relevant for Black youth, and research needs to understand the effect it has on other dimensions of well-being in addition to depression and suicidality.

Present Study

The purpose of this study is to examine the relevance of discrimination to internalizing behaviors, including anxiety/depression, suicidal thoughts, and social-emotional development, specifically emotion regulation, and prosocial behavior. In addition, while research has been conducted predominantly in large metropolitan Black-majority communities (e.g., Seaton & Yip, 2009), less empirical data is available on Black youth living in less densely populated areas. To fill this gap, this study utilizes data from Black youth that lived in small cities and rural areas.

The current study assessed how discrimination experienced in the past year was associated with higher levels of internalizing problems, specifically anxiety/depression and suicidal thoughts, and lower levels of social-emotional development, including emotion regulation and prosocial behavior. Next, we tested whether anticipation of discrimination contributed to similar patterns among Black youth. Finally, we tested whether discriminatory experiences or expectations had independent effects on youth outcomes.

Method

The data for this project came from three communities across two southern USA counties involved in implementing a community-systems intervention called Evidence2Success®. The Evidence2Success framework is a service-delivery system that brings together stakeholders in systems and communities to select and integrate tested and effective programs into communities based on youth-reported risk and protective factors, and outcomes (Annie E. Casey Foundation, n.d.). The current study does not focus on the implementation of the Evidence2Success framework, but rather used secondary data analysis on the youth assessment to explore the stated research questions. The unique census sample of 10th- and 12th-grade Black youth, with the combination of data needed to address these research questions, positioned us to examine our research questions. The analysis, no. 19–531, was given Non-Human Subject Research Determination by Southern Illinois University School of Medicine.

Participants

Youth (n = 1,860 students) in 10th and 12th grades responded to the Youth Experience Survey. Only the data from youth who identified as Black/African American were included in the analysis. Specifically, youth were asked, “Please choose the ONE answer that BEST describes what you consider yourself to be: (a) White, (b) Black, African American or African, (c) Hispanic, Latino or Spanish origin, (d) American Indian or Alaska Native, (e) Asian, (f) Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander, (g) Multiracial or biracial, or (h) Other.” Across eight schools, 1435 Black youth or 70.15% of the total sample (56.57% female; 56.40% 10th grade) 10th and 12th grades responded to the Youth Experience Survey.

Measures

Control Variables

Site location was entered into step 1 as a covariate.

Independent Variables

Three variables were included in step 2 for each analysis: age, gender, and racial discrimination (experienced or expected or both). See Table ​Table11 for descriptive statistics.

Table 1

Correlation matrix of independent, dependent, and control variables

VariableNMSD345678910
3. Age (centered)1155----------
4. Gender (female)1152--−.01-------
5. Experienced discrimination (centered)11551.311.54.10***−.07*------
6. Expected discrimination (centered)11491.381.09.05−.07*.39***-----
7. Anxiety/depression11323.262.69−.04.34***.05.04----
8. Suicidal thoughts52417.94****-.04.09.11.19***.35***---
9. Prosocial behavior11356.792.53.02.20***−.06*−.10**.16***−.02--
10. Emotion regulation11326.962.99.06*−.16***−.09**−.12***−.28***−.21***.22***-

Open in a separate window

Out of 524 youth, 94 (17.94%) reported suicidal thoughts in the past 12months. The sample size for this item is lower than the others because one community excluded the item from their survey

* < .05; ** < .01; *** < .001. ****

Experienced Discrimination

Two items examined first-hand recollections of racial discrimination in the past 12months (r = 0.39): (a) “In the past year (12months); how often have you been treated badly by other people because of your race?” (b) “In the past year (12months), how much negative discrimination have you experienced?” Items were rated on a 4-point scale (i.e., 0 = never; 3 = often). These two items were originally part of a 3-item scale on racism and discrimination designed by the Social Development Research Group for the Seattle Social Development Project Surveys. However, these were separated out by the current authors to form an “experienced discrimination” scale and an item about “expectations of discrimination” due to differences in time where discrimination either took place in the past year or was anticipated.

Expected Discrimination

One item on a 4-point scale assessed how much youth thought their race may affect their prospects, “Do you think it will be harder for you to get ahead in life because of your race?” from 0 (not at all harder) to 3 (a lot harder).

Gender

Gender was coded as a binary variable (0 = male; and 1 = female).

Age

Age was a continuous variable in years.

Dependent Variables

Four dependent variables were included in the analysis: (a) anxiety/depression, (b) suicidal thoughts, (c) prosocial behavior, (d) and emotion regulation.

Anxiety/Depression

The Anxiety/Depression scale was originally the full Emotional Symptoms 5-item subscale from the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. It was renamed in Evidence2Success to capture anxiety and depression. Students were given statements and asked to rank how true the manifestations of anxiety and depression were within the last 12months (a = 0.80). Responses ranged on a 3-point scale from 0 (Not true) to 2 (Certainly). The language in the statements was as follows: (a) I get a lot of headaches, stomachaches, or sickness; (b) I worry a lot; (c) I am often unhappy, depressed, or tearful; (d) I am nervous in new situations, I easily lose confidence; and (e) I have many fears, I am easily scared.

Suicidal Thoughts

The item was based on the 2011 Youth Risk and Behavioral Surveillance Survey from the Center for Disease Control. The original item read “During the past 12months, did you ever seriously consider attempting suicide?” The item was modified to “During the past year (12months), have you seriously thought about killing yourself? And better fit with the question leads to the rest of the survey. Students responded with a “yes” or “no.”

Prosocial Behavior

The entire 5-item subscale “prosocial behavior” came from the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (a = 0.82): (a) I try to be nice to other people. I care about their feelings; (b) I usually share with others, (c) I am helpful if someone is hurt, upset, or feeling ill; (d) I am kind to younger children; and (e) I often volunteer to help others (parents, teachers, children). Students rated items on a 3-point scale from 0 (Not true) to 2 (Certainly true).

Emotion Regulation

Finally, the emotional regulation scale is derived from the International Youth Development Study (2003) developed by the Social Development Research Group. Students reported their level of agreement to four statements about their perceived ability to control their emotions (a = 0.80). Items were rated on a 4-point scale from 0 (No!) to 3 (Yes!). The four statements were as follows: (1) “I know how to relax when I feel tense”; (2) “I am always able to keep my feelings under control”; (3) “I know how to calm down when I am feeling nervous”; and (4) “I control my temper when people are angry with me.”

Procedure

Guardians were informed about the survey and had the option to abstain from participation. Students in 10th grade and 12th grade across eight schools were administered the Youth Experience Survey during the school day in a single testing session. For one testing site, data collection was in the fall, and for the other two sites, data collection was in the spring. The schools had a majority black student population (M = 88.19%, SD = 18.17%) which ranged from 56.00 to 100% Black. Students did not receive any direct benefits for their participation.

Data Integrity

Data were examined using IBM SPSS Statistics 28.0.0.0. Before data analysis began, data were inspected for the integrity of responses. The youth responded to an item that was used as a check for honesty: “how honest were you in filling out this survey?” Responses ranged from 1 (very honest) to 5 (not honest at all). Only data from youth who responded 1(very honest) or 2 (pretty honest) was retained for the final analysis. Of the total participants, 12.06% (173 people) did not complete the item, and 7.37% (106) people were filtered out because they selected 3 (honest sometimes) or 4 (honest occasionally). This left a total sample size of (n = 1156, 56.6% female). The respondents in the final dataset were M = 16.57 (SD = 1.18) years old.

Plan of Analyses

Three models were created for each outcome of the four outcomes: internalizing problems (anxiety/depression, suicidal thoughts) and social-emotional development (prosocial behavior, emotion regulation). A series of either hierarchical linear or hierarchical logistic regressions were performed where step 1 controlled for site and step 2 tested how age, gender, experienced discrimination, and the two interaction terms were associated with internalizing problems (anxiety/depression, suicidal thoughts) and social-emotional development (prosocial behavior, emotion regulation). Then another series of analyses were performed to test how after controlling for site, how age, gender, expected discrimination, and its interaction terms also contributed to the same outcomes. Finally, another series used the same models but combined experienced and expected discrimination together in the same step to test whether one form of discrimination contributed more to each of the four outcomes than the other. See Table ​Table22 for analyses on experienced discrimination, Table ​Table33 for analyses of expected discrimination experiences, and Table ​Table44 for the combined model.

Table 2

Hierarchical multiple regressions and hierarchical logistic regression models for experienced discrimination

ScaleVariableCumulativeSimultaneous
R2 changeSig. F changeBSEp valuelower CI (95%)upper CI (95%)
Anxiety/DepressionStep 1.002.104
Site A
Site B
Step 2.12< .001
Age−0.070.07.264−0.200.06
Gender1.860.15< .0011.562.15
Experienced discrimination0.120.05.0310.030.22
Suicidal ThoughtsStep 1
Site AN/AN/A
Site B
Step 2
Age0.11.10.2740.911.37
Gender0.55.24.0231.082.78
Experienced discrimination0.19.07.0051.061.39
Prosocial BehaviorStep 1.00.867
Site A
Site B
Step 2.04< .001
Age0.06.07.395−0.070.18
Gender1.06.15< .0010.721.31
Experienced discrimination−0.08.05.090−0.180.01
Emotion RegulationStep 1.00.678
Site A
Site B
Step 2.04< .001
Age0.17.08.023−0.020.32
Gender−1.01.18< .001−1.36−0.66
Experienced discrimination−0.20.06< .001−0.31−0.08

Open in a separate window

Age and Discrimination are centered. Suicidal Thoughts does not have a R2 change or F change because a hierarchical logistic regression was tested

Table 3

Hierarchical multiple regressions and hierarchical logistic regression models for expected discrimination

ScaleVariableCumulativeSimultaneous
R2 changeSig. F changeBSEp valueLower CIUpper CI
Anxiety/DepressionStep 1.00.125
Site A
Site B
Step 2.12< .001
Age−0.06.07.340−0.190.07
Gender1.86.15< .0011.562.16
Expected discrimination0.15.07.0360.010.28
Suicidal ThoughtsStep 1
Site AN/AN/A
Site B
Step 2
Age0.08.10.4660.881.32
Gender0.59.24.0161.112.90
Expected discrimination0.48.11< .0011.302.02
Prosocial BehaviorStep 1.00.871
Site A
Site B
Step 2.05< .001
Age0.05.06.426−0.080.17
Gender0.99.15< .0010.691.28
Expected discrimination−0.20.07.004−0.33−0.61
Emotion RegulationStep 1.00.691
Site A
Site B
Step 2.05< .001
Age0.15.76.0470.000.30
Gender−1.06.18< .001−1.41−0.72
Expected discrimination−0.37.08< .001−0.53−0.21

Open in a separate window

Age and expected discrimination are centered. Suicidal Thoughts does not have an R2 change or F change because a hierarchical logistic regression was tested

Table 4

Hierarchical multiple regressions and hierarchical logistic regression models for both discrimination types

ScaleVariableCumulativeSimultaneous
R2 changeSig. F changeBSEp valueLower CI (95%)Upper CI (95%)
Anxiety/DepressionStep 1.00.125
Site A
Site B
Step 2.12< .001
Age−0.07.07.269−0.200.06
Gender1.87.15< .0011.572.15
Experienced0.09.05.095−0.020.19
Expected0.10.08.186−0.050.25
Suicidal ThoughtsStep 1
Site AN/AN/A
Site B
Step 2
Age0.07.11.4860.881.32
Gender0.61.25.0131.142.98
Experienced0.09.08.2310.941.27
Expected0.43.12< .0011.221.95
Prosocial BehaviorStep 1.00.871
Site A
Site B
Step 2.05< .001
Age0.06.07.387−0.070.18
Gender0.98.15< .0010.691.28
Experienced−0.04.05.402−0.150.06
Expected−0.17.07.020−.032−0.03
Emotion RegulationStep 1.00.691
Site A
Site B
Step 2.05< .001
Age0.17.08.0290.0170.32
Gender−1.08.18< .001−1.43−0.73
Experienced−0.13.06.034−0.25−0.01
Expected−0.30.09< .001−0.47−0.13

Open in a separate window

Age and discrimination are centered. Suicidal Thoughts does not have an R2 change or F change because a hierarchical logistic regression was tested

Participants’ experiences with racism were lower than what has been reported in other studies (Appendix). While 71.11% expected their race to make it harder to get ahead in life, 31.57% said they were treated badly because of their race, and 45.10% said they experienced negative discrimination. Descriptive statistics for all measures are presented in Table ​Table1.1. Bivariate correlations investigated the simple associations among variables, including whether age and gender needed to be controlled for in the main analyses (see Table ​Table1).1). Results indicated that gender significantly positively correlated with anxiety/depression and prosocial behavior, but it correlated negatively with emotion regulation. In addition, age correlated positively with emotion regulation. Consequently, age and experienced discrimination were centered to account for multicollinearity, and interaction terms between age × experienced discrimination, and gender × experienced discrimination were created. The same process was done for expected discrimination. No significant interactions were detected for any of the four emotional state measures. Consequently, the reported main results are based on the regression analyses conducted without the hypothesized interaction terms in each model.

Results

Internalizing Problems

Anxiety/Depression

First, a hierarchical linear regression tested whether age, gender, and experienced discrimination were uniquely associated with higher anxiety/depression scores. Results showed that the model significantly accounted for 12.25% of the variance in outcome scores, [F (5, 1121) = 31.31, p < 0.001] (Table ​(Table2).2). Gender [B = 1.86, p < 0.001] and experienced discrimination [B = 0.12, p = 0.013] were significantly associated with anxiety/depression in Black youth. In other words, anxiety/depression was significantly higher for girls than for boys, and as experienced discrimination increased for Black youth, so did anxiety and depression.

Next, another hierarchical linear regression tested whether age, gender, and expected discrimination were uniquely associated with higher anxiety/depression scores. This model was also significant and accounted for 12.20% of the variance in outcome scores, [F(5, 1120) = 30.99, p < 0.001] (Table ​(Table3).3). Similar patterns were found to the model with expected discrimination in that both gender [B = 1.86, p < 0.001] and expected discrimination [B = 0.15, p = 0.036] were significantly associated with anxiety/depression.

Finally, a third hierarchical linear regression combined both discrimination variables into the same model and step, along with age and gender, to test whether one form of discrimination predicted anxiety/depression more than the other. This model was also significant and accounted for 12.42% of the variance in outcome scores, [F(6, 1114) = 26.33, p < 0.001] (Table ​(Table4).4). While gender [B = 1.87, p < 0.001] was still significantly associated, neither experienced nor expected discrimination significantly contributed when both were added into the model.

Suicidal Thoughts

Hierarchical binary logistic regression was used to test associations between independent variables and suicidal thoughts. The overall model was found to be statistically significant (χ2 (4) = 14.04, p = 0.007), with Nagelkerke’s R2 value of 0.04. Age [χ2 (1) = 1.20, p = 0.274, OR = 1.12, 95% CI = 0.91–1.37] was not statistically significant. However, gender [χ2 (1) = 5.15, p = 0.023, OR = 1.72, 95% CI = 1.08–1.37] and experienced discrimination [χ2 (1) = 7.78, p = 0.005, OR = 1.21, 95% CI = 1.06 – 1.39] were found to be statistically significant in predicting the odds of suicide ideation.

When exchanging experienced discrimination with expected discrimination, similar results were found again. The model was found to be statistically significant (χ2 (4) = 26.80, p < 0.001), with Nagelkerke’s R2 value of 0.08. Age [χ2 (1) = 0.53, p = 0.466, OR = 1.08, 95% CI = 0.88–1.32] was not statistically significant. However, gender [χ2 (1) = 5.76, p = 0.016, OR = 1.80, 95% CI = 1.11–2.89] and expected discrimination (χ2 (1) = 18.50, p < 0.001, OR = 1.62, 95% CI = 1.30–2.02) were found to be statistically significant in predicting the odds of suicide ideation.

Then, when combining experienced and expected discrimination in the same model, the impact of the variables diverged. The model was found to be statistically significant (χ2 (5) = 27.70, p < 0.001), with Nagelkerke’s R2 value of 0.09.Age [χ2 (1) = 0.49, p = 0.486, OR = 1.08, 95% CI = 0.88–1.32] was not statistically significant. However, gender [χ2 (1) = 6.20, p = 0.013, OR = 1.84, 95% CI = 1.14–2.89] and expected discrimination [χ2 (1) = 12.82, p < 0.001, OR = 1.54, 95% CI = 1.22–1.95] were statistically significant, but experienced discrimination was not.

Social-Emotional Development

Prosocial Behavior

Age, gender, and experienced discrimination accounted for 4.31% of the variance in prosocial behavior scores in a hierarchical linear regression, [F(5, 1124) = 10.31, p < 0.001]. Only gender was associated with prosocial behavior [B = 1.02, p < 0.001].

However, the patterns changed with expected discrimination. The model accounted for 4.74% of the variance in prosocial behavior scores [F(5, 1123) = 11.13, p < 0.001] with both gender [B = 0.99, p < 0.001] and expected discrimination [B =  −0.0.20, p = 0.004] significantly contributing to the variance in those scores.

When both experienced and expected discrimination were added in the same model in the same step, the same patterns emerged as in the suicide ideation model. The model accounted for 4.80% of the variance in prosocial behavior scores [F(6, 1117) = 9.39, p < 0.001] with both gender [B = 0.99, p < 0.001] and expected discrimination [B =  −0.0.17, p = 0.020] significantly contributing to the variance in those scores, but not experienced discrimination.

Emotion Regulation

In the final set of hierarchical linear regressions, the first model significantly accounted for 4.00% of variance in emotion regulation scores, [F(5, 1121) = 9.29, p < 0.001]. All three variables were uniquely and significantly associated with emotion regulation scores. Age [B = 0.17, p < 0.028], gender [B =  −1.01, p < 0.001], and experienced discrimination significantly associated with emotion regulation [B =  −0.20, p < 0.001].

When expected discrimination was swapped into the model, the second model accounted for 5.00% of emotion regulation scores [F(5, 1115) = 11.62, p < 0.001]. All three predictors including age [B = 0.15, p = 0.047], gender [B =  −1.06, p < 0.001], and expected discrimination [B =  −0.37, p < 0.001] significantly explained outcome scores with similar patterns.

Finally, when both experienced and expected discrimination were added into the model, the model accounted for 5.33% of emotion regulation scores [F(6, 1114) = 10.46, p < 0.001]. All four predictors, namely, age [B = 0.17, p = 0.029], gender [B =  −1.08, p < 0.001], experienced discrimination [B =  −0.13, p < 0.034], and expected discrimination [B =  −0.30, p < 0.001], significantly explained outcome scores.

Discussion

This study aimed to replicate and expand prior research that connected health and wellness outcomes with experiences of racial discrimination in Black youth (Kirkinis et al., 2018; Seaton & Yip, 2009; Seaton et al., 2010). The first goal of this investigation was to reexamine the association between racial discrimination and internalizing problems. In line with previous research (e.g., Seaton & Yip, 2009), these data suggest that experienced racial discrimination had small effects (Sullivan & Feinn, 2012) positively associated with anxiety/depression for Black youth. This paper also aimed to retest for a positive association between experienced racial discrimination and suicidal thoughts. Though the sample size for this item was lower than the others because one community excluded the item from their survey, descriptive statistics were in line with national trends (Lindsey et al., 2019); around 18% of youth had suicidal thoughts in the past 12months. Results found that the more often youth experienced racial discrimination, the more likely they were to contemplate suicide. Though the effect of racial discrimination on suicidal thoughts was small (Sullivan & Feinn, 2012), the data reaffirmed previous research on Black youth (Walker et al., 2017).

Investigations on the association between these experiences and mental health have primarily focused on negative emotional states. However, the second goal of this investigation was to add to the literature by understanding the association between experienced racial discrimination and social-emotional development. This was achieved by examining the association of experienced racial discrimination with prosocial behavior and emotion regulation. Prosocial behavior (Raposa et al., 2016) and emotion regulation (Troy & Mauss, 2011) can serve as protective factors or a skillset to ameliorate challenges encountered in life which can ultimately lead to better health outcomes. This was the first study of its kind to examine the relationship between racial discrimination and prosocial development in both Black boys and Black girls. Results found a negative association between experienced racial discrimination and emotion regulation but no significant relationship between experienced racial discrimination and prosocial behavior. The finding on prosocial behavior thus neither supports the hypothesis nor previous research with male adolescents (Lozada et al., 2017).

The current study also examined whether the patterns for expected discrimination were similar to experienced discrimination. The relationship between thoughts on how race can impact future opportunities and youth well-being is not well understood. It is important to highlight in the results that a much higher proportion of youth expected that their race would stop them from getting ahead in life in the future (71.11%) in contrast to those who experienced discrimination (31.57–45.10%). In regression analyses, experienced discrimination and expectations around discrimination did not produce the same patterns when combined into a model. When it came to anxiety and depression, the two forms of discrimination appeared to cancel each other out and account for the same variance in explaining the outcome. However, when it came to suicide ideation and prosocial behavior, only expectations around discrimination explained the outcomes. Finally, for emotion regulation, both forms of discrimination explained the outcome scores, but expected discrimination had a greater association with the outcome. Thus, what these results shed light on is that it may in fact be valuable to separate out different forms of discrimination to understand the impact they have on youth outcomes. It is interesting that while experiences and expectations explained anxiety and depression in a similar way, expectations were particularly important for other youth outcomes, like emotion regulation, which may question the long-term effects of discrimination in explaining youths emotions and their interactions with others.

Lastly, this study explored how age and gender moderated the relationship between racial discrimination and emotional states in Black youth. While no significant interactions were detected, there were main effects for both age and gender. In line with previous research, which shows that emotion regulation increases during adolescence (Zimmerman & Iwanski, 2014), age was positively associated with emotion regulation. It is possible that there were no interactions for age because there was not a wide enough range to detect age-related differences.

Main effects for gender were detected in all analyses. Specifically, scores were higher for Black girls than Black boys on every measure except emotion regulation. The lack of significant interactions between gender and either experienced or expected racial discrimination suggests that discrimination may not impact boys and girls differently, which is counter to what is presented in the news (e.g., Badger et al., 2018) and in research (e.g., Kwate & Goodman, 2015) that discrimination is experienced by boys more.

Limitations

These findings need to be considered in the context of some limitations. First, could be the use of single-item assessment. Both suicidal thoughts and expected discrimination were captured with a single item. The value in a single-item assessment is that it permits more time to capture other constructs and can reduce the chances of fatigue on participants. Additionally, research supports that many single-item assessments are in agreement with their multi-item subscales (Verster et al., 2021). However, according to Verster and colleagues, single items do not often have good agreement with full scales that tend to by multifaceted with several subscales. Additionally, they are not ideal in situations of diagnosing patients. For instance, one study found that a single item on suicide ideation can overestimate those truly experiencing suicidal thoughts by 8% (Millner et al., 2015). However, given that this study was not designed for patient intervention, having a single item capture suicide ideation is less of a concern.

A third limitation is the cross-sectional nature of the design. A longitudinal design would have permitted the test in a mediation model to understand how something such as social-emotional development could mediate the relationship between different forms of discrimination and poor mental health outcomes. The use of a cross-sectional design could have obscured the direction of effects implied by the regression analyses. For instance, while the results support that expected discrimination could be detrimental for both internalizing problems and social-emotional development it is also possible that internalizing problems and challenges with social-emotional development might increase the concern that one’s race might interfere in their future.

Implications for Community Prevention Systems

Data from this study support that racial discrimination is a risk factor not only for poor mental health outcomes like suicidal thoughts, but it also impacts protective factors around social-emotional development that mitigate negative daily experiences. Consequently, this study validates the importance of any needs assessment or epidemiological survey examining a broad range of risks/protection/and outcomes. For instance, the website Blueprints for Healthy Youth and Development (https://www.blueprintsprograms.org/) was broadened from focusing only on violence prevention to five outcome domains: problem behavior, education, physical health, emotional well-being, and positive relationships. The website organizes information such that it specifies which risk and protective factors are associated with these outcomes, highlighting the importance of this approach. Therefore, programs addressing racial discrimination should consider how risk and protective factors can relate when addressing youth mental health.

These findings also reinforce the importance in addressing systemic racism. Youth from the current study were from majority Black communities which could in part explain why youth were less optimistic about their future than those who experienced discrimination first hand. Yet, despite this, there still appears to be a consistently negative impact of racial discrimination on a range of outcomes. Future studies should focus on communities where Black youth are not the largest minority group to better understand how discrimination impact mental health outcomes. While evidence-based programs could use a risk and protective factor approach to improve youth outcomes, it is best to address the root cause: racial discrimination in the system. Brody et al. (2006) suggest that “those at the receiving end of discrimination can, overtime, come to internalize the discrimination view” (p. 1183). In addition, since the context in which these discriminatory experiences occurred was not asked about, it is challenging to know if youth in this study experienced discrimination in community settings, such as retail stores or schools, where they are likely to encountered non-Black personnel. However, regardless of which systems experiences took place, the data from this study demonstrates that expectations about systemic racism can have an equally detrimental impact on adolescents’ mental well-being.

The push for equitable services must continue to persist. It is impossible to predict the magnitude that eliminating discriminatory experiences for Black youth could have on their mental health and life trajectory. It could begin with youth connecting more to their community or having increased self-worth. However, in the long term, it may lead to reduced incarceration, homelessness, comorbidities, and even Medicare spending.

Future Directions

These findings lend to future considerations around measurement quality, moderators, and the developmental impact of discrimination. The frequency and intensity of racially motivated experiences is only going to be detected to the extent that the measure tolerates. In a meta-analysis on the impact of discrimination on well-being during adolescence, 40 measures on racial discrimination were used (Benner et al., 2018). Therefore, just as it was suggested in Benner et al., more measurement work is still needed to understand the complexity of racial discrimination (e.g., timing, perpetrator, experienced vs. witnessed). For instance, the impact may be different whether these interactions are with peers, administrators, sales associates, or law enforcement. Future research on this topic should also employ cross-sectional and longitudinal designs. These types of designs could elucidate how events (e.g., Brianna Taylor) impact youth both in the immediate and long terms, but also to better understand the cumulative effect of perceived systematic discriminatory incidents in the lived experiences of school-aged youth. These approaches would establish developmental approaches that better serve younger children, younger adolescents, older adolescents, and youth transitioning to adulthood in dealing with the deleterious impacts of discrimination on well-being and identity. In addition, more research is needed on how experiences of discrimination shape youths’ cognitions about their self-efficacy so that it can be addressed in interventions until health equity is created.

Research needs to investigate the impact of racial discrimination over the lifespan. In other words, are there differential outcomes for youth who report lower levels of racial discrimination during the adolescent years compared to youth that report higher levels of racial discrimination? Similarly, it should not be assumed that the types of racial discrimination experienced during adolescence are the same experienced as youth transition to adulthood or in stages of adulthood. Racial discrimination in public schools may be qualitatively different than those experiences at college/university or the workforce. Longitudinal research may facilitate an understanding, and this work needs to involve both quantitative and qualitative approaches to better understand the life impact of discrimination for the Black community.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Mildred Johnson, Lisa Gary, and Kantahyanee Murray for their valuable feedback on this paper.

Appendix. Proportion of Black Adolescents who Experienced Discrimination

Sample sizeParticipant age (years)Scale nameLength of reflectionProportion
Sellers et al. (2006)314M = 13.8; range = 11–17Daily Life Experiences ScalePast 12months62.5%
Seaton et al. (2008)810M = 15; range = 13–17Everyday Discrimination ScalePast 12months87%
Dulin-Keita et al. (2011)57M = 9.72; range = 7–12Williams Every-Day Discrimination Scale (racial discrimination subscale)Past 30days19.99%
Roberts et al. (2012)745M = 10.5; range = 10–11Schedule of Racist EventsGeneral experiences38% (more than minimal experience); 89% (some experience)
McNeil Smith and Fincham (2015)711M = 14.24; range = 13–17School Discrimination ScaleGeneral experiences22–40%
Nagata et al. (2021)1656M = N/A; range 10–11Perceived Discrimination ScaleGeneral experiences10%
Current Paper1156M = 16.57; range = 14–19Experienced Discrimination; Expected DiscriminationPast 12months; general experiencesExperienced (48.83%); expected (71.11%)

Open in a separate window

Funding

The data collection was funded directly to the community partners by the Annie E. Casey Foundation. We thank them for their support but acknowledge that the findings and conclusions presented in this report are those of the author(s) alone, and do not necessarily reflect the opinions of the Foundation.

Data Availability

If you would like access to this data, we will work with our institutions and the communities involved in data collection to make the data as available as we can with appropriate protections.

Declarations

Ethics Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. This IRB protocol number assigned to this study for raw data collection was by Washington University no. 40396 and then later became STUDY00001085 after conversion to the Zipline system. The Southern Illinois University School of Medicine IRB gave this study (19–531) a Non-Human Subjects Research Determination.

Informed Consent

For this type of study, formal consent was not required.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

The original online version of this article was revised due to a retrospective Open Access order.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • Annie E. Casey Foundation (2023). Evidence2Success: Involving communities in assessing and improving the well-being of children and young people.https://www.aecf.org/work/community-change/evidence2success/
  • Assari S, Mistry R, Lee DB, Caldwell CH, Zimmerman MA. Perceived racial discrimination and marijuana use a decade later; Gender differences among black youth. Frontiers in Pediatrics. 2019;7(78):1–11. doi:10.3389/fped.2019.00078. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Badger, E., Miller, C. C., Pearce, A., & Quealy, K. (2018). Extensive data shows punishing reach of racism for black boys. New York Times, 19.
  • Benner AD, Wang Y, Shen Y, Boyle AE, Polk R, Cheng YP. Racial/ethnic discrimination and well-being during adolescence: A meta-analytic review. American Psychologist. 2018;73(7):855–883. doi:10.1037/amp0000204. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Brody GH, Chen YF, Murry VM, Ge X, Simons RL, Gibbons FX, Gerrard M, Cutrona CE. Perceived discrimination and the adjustment of African American youths: A five-year longitudinal analysis with contextual moderation effects. Child Development. 2006;77(5):1170–1189. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00927.x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Copping KE, Jurtz-Costes B, Rowley SJ, Wood D. Age and race differences in racial stereotype awareness and endorsem*nt. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2013;43(5):971–980. doi:10.1111/jasp.12061. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Cunningham M, Mulser RM, Scott K, Yates A. African American adolescents speak: the meaning of racial identity in the relation between individual race-related stress and depressive symptoms. In: Fitzgerald HE, Johnson DJ, Qin DB, Villarruel FA, Norder J, editors. Handbook of children and prejudice: integrating research, practice, and policy. Switzerland: Springer; 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • Davis AN, McGinley M, Carlo G, Schwartz SJ, Unger JB, Rosiers SED, Baezconde-Garbanati L, Lorenzo-Blanco EI, Soto D. Examining discrimination and familism values as longitudinal predictors of prosocial behaviors among recent immigrant adolescents. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2021;45(4):317–326. doi:10.1177/01650254211005561. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Davis AN, Carlo G. Handbook of children and prejudice. Cham: Springer; 2019. Toward an integrative conceptual model on the relations between discrimination and prosocial behaviors in US Latino/Latina youth; pp. 375–388. [Google Scholar]
  • Dulin-Keita A, Hannon Iii L, Fernandez JR, co*ckerham WC. The defining moment: Children's conceptualization of race and experiences with racial discrimination. Ethnic and Racial Studies. 2011;34(4):662–682. doi:10.1080/01419870.2011.535906. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Herda D. The specter of discrimination: Fear of interpersonal racial discrimination among adolescents in Chicago. Social Science Research. 2016;55:48–62. doi:10.1016/j.ssresearch.2015.09.010. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • International Youth Development Study (IYDS) Student Survey (2003). Social Development Research Group, University of Washington, Seattle. http://www.sdrg.org/current.asp
  • Kirkinis K, Pieterse AL, Martin C, Agiliga A, Brownell A. Racism, racial discrimination, and trauma: A systematic review of the social science literature. Ethnicity and Health. 2018;45:232–259. doi:10.1080/13557858.2018.1514453. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kwate NOA, Goodman MS. Racism at the intersections: Gender and socioeconomic differences in the experience of racism among African Americans. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 2015;85(5):397–408. doi:10.1037/ort0000086. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Lindsey, M. A., Sheftall, A. H., Xiao, Y., & Joe, S. (2019). Trends of suicidal behaviors among high school students in the United States: 1991–2017.Pediatrics,144(5). 10.1542/peds.2019-1187 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Lozada FT, Jagers RJ, Smith CD, Bañales J, Hope EC. Prosocial behaviors of Black adolescent boys: An application of a sociopolitical development theory. Journal of Black Psychology. 2017;43(5):493–516. doi:10.1177/0095798416652021. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • McNeil Smith S, Fincham F. Racial discrimination experiences among black youth. Journal of Black Psychology. 2015;42(4):300–319. doi:10.1177/0095798415573315. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Millner AJ, Lee MD, Nock MK. Single-item measurement of suicidal behaviors: Validity and consequences of misclassification. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(10):1–17. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0141606. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Nagata, J. M., Ganson, K. T., Sajjad, O. M., Benabou, S. E., & Bibbins-Domingo, K. (2021). Prevalence of perceived racism and discrimination among US children aged 10 and 11 years: The adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) Study.JAMA pediatrics. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.1022 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Padilla-Walker LM, Carlo G, Christensen KJ, Yorgason JB. Bidirectional relations between authoritative parenting and adolescents’ prosocial behaviors. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2012;22(3):400–408. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2012.00807.x. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Raposa EB, Laws HB, Ansell EB. Prosocial behavior mitigates the negative effects of stress in everyday life. Clinical Psychological Science. 2016;4(4):691–698. doi:10.1177/2167702615611073. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Riley TN, DeLaney E, Brown D, Lozada FT, Williams CD, Dick DM. The associations between African American emerging adults’ racial discrimination and civic engagement via emotion regulation. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2021;27(2):169. doi:10.1037/cdp0000335. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Roberts ME, Gibbons FX, Gerrard M, Weng C-Y, Murry VM, Simons LG, Simons RL, Lorenz FO. From racial discrimination to risky sex: Prospective relations involving peers and parents. Developmental Psychology. 2012;48(1):89–102. doi:10.1037/a0025430. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Seaton EK, Yip T. School and neighborhood contexts, perceptions of racial discrimination, and psychological well-being among African American adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2009;38(2):153–163. doi:10.1007/s10964-008-9356-x. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Seaton EK, Caldwell CH, Sellers RM, Jackson JS. The prevalence of perceived discrimination amongAfrican American and Caribbean Black youth. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44(5):1288–1297. doi:10.1037/a0012747. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Seaton EK, Caldwell CH, Sellers RM, Jackson JS. An intersectional approach for understanding perceived discrimination and psychological well-being among African American and Caribbean black youth. Developmental Psychology. 2010;46(5):1372–1379. doi:10.1037/a0019869. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Sellers RM, Linder NC, Martin PM, Lewis RL. Racial identity matters: The relation between racial discrimination and psychological functioning in African American adolescents. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2006;16(2):187–216. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2006.00128.x. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Sullivan GM, Feinn R. Using effect size—or why the P value is not enough. Journal of Graduate Medical Education. 2012;4(3):279–282. doi:10.4300/JGME-D-12-00156.1. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Taylor RD, Oberle E, Durlak JA, Weissberg RP. Promoting positive youth development through school-based social and emotional learning interventions: A meta-analysis of follow-up effects. Child Development. 2017;88(4):1156–1171. doi:10.1111/cdev.12864. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Troy AS, Mauss IB. Resilience in the face of stress: Emotion regulation as a protective factor. In: Southwick SM, Litz BT, Charney D, Friedman M, editors. Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges across the Lifespan. Cambridge University Press; 2011. pp. 30–44. [Google Scholar]
  • Umaña-Taylor AJ. A post-racial society in which ethnic-racial discrimination still exists and has significant consequences for youths' adjustment. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2016;25(2):111–118. doi:10.1177/0963721415627858. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Verster JC, Sandalova E, Garssen J, Bruce G. The use of single-item ratings versus traditional multiple-item questionnaires to assess mood and health. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education. 2021;11(1):183–198. doi:10.3390/ejihpe11010015. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Walker RL, Salami TK, Carter SE, Flowers K. Perceived racism and suicide ideation: Mediating role of depression but moderating role of religiosity among African American adults. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior. 2014;44(5):548–559. doi:10.1111/sltb.12089. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Walker R, Francis D, Brody G, Simons R, Cutrona C, Gibbons F. A longitudinal study of racial discrimination and risk for death ideation in African American youth. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior. 2017;47(1):86–102. doi:10.1111/sltb.12251. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Williams DR, Lawrence JA, Davis BA. Racism and health: Evidence and needed research. Annual Review of Public Health. 2019;40:105–125. doi:10.1146/annurev-publhealth-040218-043750. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Zimmermann P, Iwanski A. Emotion regulation from early adolescence to emerging adulthood and middle adulthood: Age differences, gender differences, and emotion-specific developmental variations. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2014;38(2):182–194. doi:10.1177/0165025413515405. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Zimmerman, G. M., & Miller-Smith, A. (2021). The impact of anticipated, vicarious, and experienced racial and ethnic discrimination on depression and suicidal behavior among Chicago youth. Social Science Research, 102623. 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2021.102623 [PubMed]

Articles from Prevention Science are provided here courtesy of Springer

Discriminatory Experiences Among Black Youth: How Encounters and Expectations Explain Emotional Well-Being (2024)
Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Sen. Ignacio Ratke

Last Updated:

Views: 5705

Rating: 4.6 / 5 (56 voted)

Reviews: 95% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Sen. Ignacio Ratke

Birthday: 1999-05-27

Address: Apt. 171 8116 Bailey Via, Roberthaven, GA 58289

Phone: +2585395768220

Job: Lead Liaison

Hobby: Lockpicking, LARPing, Lego building, Lapidary, Macrame, Book restoration, Bodybuilding

Introduction: My name is Sen. Ignacio Ratke, I am a adventurous, zealous, outstanding, agreeable, precious, excited, gifted person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.